Skip to main content

Emphasis

Emphasis is a fundamental aspect of language and plays a great part in determining the basic structure of the syntax. Unfortunately, most visions of grammar underestimate this importance; nevertheless, it is the important tool which is not only used to stress one part of an idea or another but also transfers the weight of importance of the phrase from one part of the structure to another through inversions, etc. To discuss the English verb system without an understanding of the importance of emphasis would be to miss several important and fundamental arguments about structure, logic and interpretation.

Vocal and Structural Emphasis

There are two basic forms of emphasis: vocal and structural emphasis. In the former, one word or another is simply given more force than the rest through emphasized speech, leaving it quite clear which word carries the greater importance:

She doesn’t like chocolate - indicates who.
She doesn’t like chocolate - emphasizes the negative.
She doesn’t like chocolate - emphasizes the verb to like and not any other action.
She doesn’t like chocolate - emphasizes what (in this case) we don't like.

Without changing the syntax (the word order) of our phrase, the use of emphasis on different words in the phrases produces differing messages. On the other hand, the use of the natural unforced syntactical emphasis throws the importance onto the word that falls before the main verb - the auxiliary or the subject pronoun for instance:

She doesn’t like chocolate
Doesn’t she like chocolate?

In the emphatic inversion of the modal doesn’t, we can see that the phrase inverts to form the interrogative. That is to say that, through the change of positions, we create the question form, which can be both positive and negative, as in our case above. Nevertheless, an emphatic inversion is exactly what it says it is and therefore can as easily be a simple use of emphasis as a question “interrogative” form. Notice the following:

She does like chocolate!
Does she like chocolate!
Does she like chocolate?

In the first example, we can see that with the modal does before the verb, the modal is emphatic. Furthermore, in the second example, the inversion of the pronoun and modal allows the emphasis to fall on the subject of the verb in an emphatic phrase. When, however, we reduce the vocal emphasis on the pronoun (she), to less than the surrounding auxiliary and verb, the phase becomes less vertically driven, more horizontal and slightly uplifted, giving us the question form, whilst still allowing us to choose which word we want to add a moderate vocal emphasis to.

Further to this, the more separated the words are, more emphatic and marked our phrase becomes, as if we were hammering them out.

Inversely, the use of contractions and contracted inversions help to horizontalize the phrase, which leaves it less marked by stress:

I am a teacher. (verticle emphasis on the verb)
I’m a teacher. ( natural slight emphatic drop on the noun)

The concept also includes inverting the modal does with our verb like and contracting it to the less emphatic form likes:

She does like chocolate! (vertical emphasis on the modal – DOES)
She likes chocolate. (contracted form makes for a smooth horizontal phrase)

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

How the Present Perfect Tense works

Timeline - How the Present Perfect Tense works Experiences The essence of the present perfect tense pulls the verbal expression down, inside us. This movement reflects our experience and even belief and true knowledge, which even in mediaeval times was placed in the heart. The sense we are aiming for is very similar in all of the following three examples: • I have visited the Louvre. • I believe in aliens. • I know it’s true. No Details The movement physically collapses our lungs. For this reason we do not go into detail, because in simple structures, we can not add extensive information. To add detail of past experiences, we typically use the simple and progressive past tense or use an adverb. • There has been a plane crash • A Transatlantic flight out of Paris crashed at 7.45 pm, twenty minutes after take off. Nobody was killed …etc. Recent Time In a similar movement - a slight variation on the theme - the expression is pulled back and released until the

Chart of Modal Inflection

With each movement and directional vector (or sense) of each modal, we can start to map out the areas of modal expression. The chart (illustrated above) shows how each aspect is directed;a skill we certainly need to emulate, if we are to give the correct language signals. As the chart shows , the high area is reserved for probability, the degree of which ranges from positive certainty through possibility to uncertainty at the back of the head. The forward area (ahead of the eye) carries the different notions of future modality, rising from the low downward driven decisive intention or acceptance (will) and determination (shall) through obligation(must), permission (may) and advice – or deferred obligation (should) to prediction (will) and desire (will, must, should). The past forms are set back or directed behind us, with past probability looped high in speculation but ending low - so as to combine with the auxiliary have and the past pa

Infinitive or Gerund - Part 1

Infinitive or Gerund - ? The area of infinitive or gerund is as much of a headache for students as it is for teachers. On the other hand, the native speaker has a perfect control over the choice. Again, it all comes down to the instinctive use of language and to how our brains have programmed the information in terms of the positional dynamic of pronunciation.  Back in the classroom, however, many teachers still use the old tedious method of list learning which essentially achieves two purposes: 1. That the teacher, having the lists, is shown to be knowledgeable. 2. That certain students will excel at list-learning, gaining confidence at the expense of others. Assuming that the area needs to be learnt in such a fashion, native speakers would need to learn in the same way and if the area was actually difficult or confusing, many natural errors would, in fact, result. Of course, in reality, no errors are made by native speakers when choosing a gerund or infinitive in